Lohr DSM-5 and ASD.txt

Plain Text icon DSM-V article.txt — Plain Text, 31 KB (32545 bytes)

File contents

FEATURE

Healio.com/Pediatrics | 161

PEDIATRIC ANNALS 42:4 | APRIL 2013

DSM-5 and Proposed Changes 
to the Diagnosis of Autism

W. David Lohr, MD; and Peter Tanguay, MD

When the American Psychiatric 
Association releases the 
Diagnostic and Statistical 
Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition 
(DSM-5) next month, children currently 
diagnosed with autism may lose access 
to services. Modifications to the proposed 
criteria have been suggested to address 
concerns of sensitivity. 

IMPLICATIONS OF DSM-5

The American Psychiatric Association�s 
stated goals for the changes to its diagnostic 
criteria for autism spectrum disorder 
(ASD) in DSM-5 are to �accurately and 
completely identify� individuals with autism 
by production of reliable and valid diagnostic 
criteria1 in order to offer a clearer, 
simpler, more reliable diagnostic scheme 
and recognize the �essential shared features 
of the autism spectrum.�2 This means 
that, for the clinician, it should be easier to 
diagnose ASD than trying to distinguish 
between high-functioning autism (HFA) 
and Asperger�s disorder. In addition, the 
vague diagnosis of pervasive developmental 
disorder not otherwise specified 
(PDD-NOS) has been replaced, in part, by 
social communication disorder (SCD) (see 
Table 1, page 162). 

Questions have already been raised 
about the appearance of a new diagnosis, 
SCD, that describes children with difficulties 
in the pragmatics of verbal and nonverbal 
communication, leading to impaired 
social function. This disorder excludes an 
autism diagnosis, and thus rules out the 
presence of restrictive, repetitive behaviors 
(RRBs);3 however, SCD seems to resemble 
mild autism or PDD-NOS.4,5 Questions 
remain about how the criteria for SCD will 
be implemented, and whether children who 
meet criteria for SCD will meet eligibility 
for medical and educational services.6

Overall, the changes should result in 
more reliable and valid diagnoses, with 
estimates of individual severity and associated 
conditions. It will remain important 
for clinicians to take a thoughtful and 
complete history and combine information 
from multiple sources with clinical observation 
to diagnose children with ASD with 
improved specificity. 

For clinicians and families, the singular 
question is whether the new criteria will 
change access to treatment and/or educational 
services. Studies have noted that individuals 
with a diagnosis of Asperger�s disorder 
or PDD-NOS, those with higher IQ, 
and female patients may be at most risk.4 
This could impact whether children with 
an established DSM-IV diagnosis of Asperger�s 
disorder or PDD-NOS would need 


W. David Lohr, MD, is Assistant Professor of 
Pediatrics, Division of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 
and Psychology; Associate in the Department 
of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences; 
and Co-Clinical Director, University of Louisville 
Autism Center, University of Louisville School 
of Medicine. Peter Tanguay, MD, is the Spafford 
Ackerly Endowed Professor of Child and Adolescent 
Psychiatry (Emeritus) in the Department of 
Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of 
Louisville School of Medicine.

Address correspondence to: W. David Lohr, 
MD, University of Louisville Autism Center, 200 
E. Chestnut Street, Louisville, KY 40202; fax: 502-
852-1055; email: wdlohr01@louisville.edu.

Disclosure: The authors have no relevant financial 
relationships to disclose.

doi: 10.3928/00904481-20130326-12

� Shutterstock



FEATURE

162 | Healio.com/Pediatrics

PEDIATRIC ANNALS 42:4 | APRIL 2013

re-evaluation per the DSM-5 criteria. The 
question then becomes whether insurance 
companies and educational systems continue 
to accept these diagnoses for coverage. 

Improved ability to identify a more homogenous 
group of ASD should help those 
researching the condition by improving 
the ability to detect etiologies, endophenotypes, 
genetic markers, and by strengthening 
treatment and outcome data. Yet, 
questions remain about how the proposed 
changes will affect the compatibility of future 
research with prior research and how 
the criteria will mesh with International 
Classification of Diseases, eleventh revision 
(ICD-11) standards.7

HISTORY OF AUTISM IN THE DSM 

Although autism was first described by 
Kanner in 1935,8 it was not considered a 
formal psychiatric diagnosis until the release 
of DSM-III in 1980.9 In this system, 
to be diagnosed with autism, individuals 
had to meet all diagnostic criteria (monothetic) 
and the categories described a condition 
similar to classical autism.4,9

In the 1987 DSM-III-R version, a subset 
with a range of criteria (polythetic) could 
be applied during diagnosis, leading to a 
more heterogeneous diagnostic group10 
DSM-IV, published in 1994 (see Table 2, 
page 163), has continued the polythetic 
approach.11 To meet the diagnostic criteria, 
patients must meet a minimum of six 
behavioral criteria subsets: two from social 
impairment; one from communication; and 
one from RRBs. Also, the onset of the condition 
must be prior to age 3 years. 

Introduction of Asperger�s Disorder

The DSM-IV requires that for an Asperger�s 
disorder diagnosis, the patient 
meet a minimum of three criteria: two from 
social impairment and one from RRB. Asperger�s 
disorder differs from autism in that 
there can be no communication impairment 
or delay in language, and no age of onset 
by 3 years. Also, there can be no delay in 
cognitive development or nonsocial adaptive 
behavior. DSM-IV criteria for PDD-
NOS are for subthreshold presentations of 
autistic symptoms featuring impairment 
in social or communicative functioning or 
RRB (see Table 2, page 163).

Concerns about the DSM-IV and ASD

Questions about how the DSM-IV addresses 
ASD led to revisions in the DSM-
5. First, the number of possible symptom 
combinations in the DSM-IV has been estimated 
to be 2,027, with inherent heterogeneity 
of the diagnostic group.4 Concerns 
exist also with its reliability and validity. In 
a recent multisite study of 2,102 probands, 
the best-estimate clinical diagnoses were 
compared with those from standardized 
diagnostic instruments.12 

Experts differed on how they interpreted 
DSM-IV criteria, even though the 
reliability of the data from the standardized 
interviews was good. Patterns of 
diagnosis were identifiable according to 
regional sites, with factors such as verbal 
IQ and language level influencing the 
process. 

Also, research has not identified meaningful 
differences between DSM-IV-text 
revision (published in 2000) subtypes of 
ASD controlled for IQ and language.13 
In particular, Asperger�s disorder is not 
thought to be distinct from HFA with little 
support of DSM-IV distinction.2,14,15 

TABLE 1. 

Comparison of Autism Criteria in DSM-IV and DSM-5

DSM-IV

DSM-5

A. (1) a. marked impairment in the use of multiple nonverbal aspects of social interaction

A2

A. (1) b. failure to develop appropriate peer relationships

A3

A. (1) c. lack of spontaneous sharing with other people

A1

A. (1) d. lack of social or emotional reciprocity

A1

A. (2) a. delay or lack of spoken language

?

A. (2) b. impaired ability to initiate or sustain a conversation

A1

A. (2) c. stereotypical and repetitive language

B1

A. (2) d. lack of make-believe or imitative play

A3

A. (3) a. stereotypical and restricted patterns of interest

B3

A. (3) b. inflexible adherence to rituals or routines

B2

A. (3) c. stereotypical and repetitive motor movements

B1

A. (3) d. persistent preoccupations with parts of objects

B3

None

B4

B. delays in at least one of three areas with onset prior to age 3 years of social interaction, language used as 
(1) social communication, (2) symbolic or (3) imaginative play, or (4) hyper-or hypo-reactivity to sensory input

A1 and A3 with onset in early childhood







FEATURE

PEDIATRIC ANNALS 42:4 | APRIL 2013

Diagnostic Challenges of Asperger�s 
Disorder

Asperger�s disorder is difficult to diagnose 
using DSM-IV criteria because 
accurately measuring language delays 
retrospectively is a challenge, as is clinically 
distinguishing these patients from 
those with autism.2 In addition, Asperger�s 
disorder diagnoses have been 
shown to be unreliable between expert 
clinicians.12 It is also important to note 
children with autism who met language 
milestones before the age of 3 years may 
have the same adult outcome as those 
children with autism who did not.16 

The DSM-IV criteria also have been 
faulted with how well they diagnose autism 
in children younger than 5 years, 
adolescents, females, and ethnic minority 
groups.1 These concerns with the limitations 
of DSM-IV have been raised over the 
last 20 years by researchers in the area of 
diagnosis and classification of autism spectrum 
disorders and have prompted the development 
of the criteria found in DSM-5.

CHANGES IN THE DSM-5

In the DSM-5, autism, Asperger�s disorder, 
and PDD-NOS will be combined into 
a single category of ASD6 and supplemented 
with a dimensional aspect for assessing 
the level of dysfunction. This is important 
because social communication function 
appears to be distributed in a continuous 
fashion across the general population.17,18

In particular, the domains for social and 
communication problems have been combined 
into one set of deficits, labeled,�social 
communication and interactive problems.� 
The set of symptoms for restricted, repetitive 
interests remains, but unusual sensory 
behaviors have been added to their diagnostic 
set. 

The DSM-5�s approach to social communication 
symptoms is monothetic, requiring 
that individuals meet all criteria 
from the social-communicative set; for 
RRB, the DSM-5 is polythetic, requiring 
that two of four symptoms be present. In 
all, five of seven symptoms must be present 
in the DSM-5, compared with six of 12 
symptoms required by the DSM-IV (see 
Table 3, page 164).

The deficits in communication and 
social behaviors were combined into one 
domain because the Autism Work Group 
of the DSM-5 Committee believed the two 
represent a similar impairment.5 Because 
a delay in language is not believed to be 
unique or universal in ASD, this criterion 
is eliminated altogether. 

The requirement for two symptom sets 
for repetitive behaviors and fixated interests 
by history or direct observation is 
thought to increase the stability of the diagnosis 
of ASD over time. The criteria now 
include symptoms for abnormal sensory 
behaviors. This improves the relevance of 
the criteria to younger children with ASD, 
because sensory issues are common concerns 
in this population. 

The development of the DSM-5 has 
been based on literature review, expert 
consultations, work group discussions, 
and secondary analysis of data sets. As 
noted, making the diagnosis should be 

TABLE 2. 

Diagnostic Criteria for 
Autistic Disorder from DSM-IV

A. A total of six (or more) items from (1), (2), and (3), with at least two from (1), and one each from 
(2) and (3):

 (1) Qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:

 (a) marked impairments in the use of multiple nonverbal behaviors such as eye-to-eye gaze, 
facial expression, body posture, and gestures to regulate social interaction;

 (b) failure to develop peer relationships appropriate to developmental level;

 (c) a lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment, interests, or achievements with other 
people, (eg, by a lack of showing, bringing, or pointing out objects of interest to other people); and

 (d) lack of social or emotional reciprocity (note: in the description, it gives the following as 
examples: not actively participating in simple social play or games, preferring solitary activities, 
or involving others in activities only as tools or �mechanical� aids).

 (2) Qualitative impairments in communication as manifested by at least one of the following:

 (a) delay in, or total lack of, the development of spoken language (not accompanied by an 
attempt to compensate through alternative modes of communication such as gesture or mime);

 (b) in individuals with adequate speech, marked impairment in the ability to initiate or 
sustain a conversation with others;

 (c) stereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic language;

 (d) lack of varied, spontaneous make-believe play or social imitative play appropriate to 
developmental level.

 (3) Restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests and activities, as manifested 
by at least two of the following:

 (a) encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of 
interest that is abnormal either in intensity or focus;

 (b) apparently inflexible adherence to specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals;

 (c) stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms (eg, hand or finger flapping or twisting, or 
complex whole-body movements);

 (d) persistent preoccupation with parts of objects.

B. Delays or abnormal functioning in at least one of the following areas, with onset prior to age 
3 years: (1) social interaction, (2) language as used in social communication, or (3) symbolic or 
imaginative play.

C. The disturbance is not better accounted for by Rett�s disorder or childhood disintegrative 
disorder.

Source: American Psychiatric Association11



FEATURE

PEDIATRIC ANNALS 42:4 | APRIL 2013

easier for the pediatrician since the criteria 
are designed to promote more agreement 
between clinicians. 

EVIDENCE FOR AND AGAINST 
DSM-5 CHANGES

Several studies have been conducted 
to determine how DSM-5 changes will 
affect the ASD population. Mattila et al19 
compared DSM-IV criteria with early draft 
criteria for the DSM-5 in 82 individuals 
derived from an epidemiological sample 
of 5,484 8-year-olds. The DSM-5 group 
was less sensitive for HFA (IQ > 70) and 
Asperger�s disorder, but the group was an 
earlier version later updated by the Neurodevelopmental 
Disorders Workgroup. For 
patients with HFA, 73% were identified 
by the DSM-5 but none of 11 subjects with 
Asperger�s disorder were identified using 
the DSM-5 criteria. 

The study�s authors suggested five 
modifications to relax the DSM-5 criteria 
and create a �mild� version of autism compared 
with the �strict version� identified 
by the DSM-5. After the authors modified 
the DSM-5 criteria, 96% of overall subjects 
were identified. No information is offered 
on effects of specificity.19

Supporting evidence was provided by 
Mandy et al20 who reported that the DSM-
5 offered improved construct validity over 
DSM-IV-TR by improving the criteria language 
and by the inclusion of hyper- and 
hyposensory abnormalities as part of the 
symptoms cluster. 

In a large analysis of siblings by Frazier 
et al21 from the Interactive Autism Network, 
8,911 siblings were found to have 
ASD; 5,863 did not. Compared with the 
DSM-IV TR, the proposed criteria show 
greater specificity to reduce false-positive 
diagnoses but slightly lower sensitivity so 
more false-negative diagnoses may result, 
especially with females. 

Frazier et al21 proposed relaxing the 
DSM-5 criteria by requiring one less symptom 
criteria of SCI or RRB to increase sensitivity 
by 11% to 12%. This may be very 
pertinent for those diagnosed with Asperger�s 
disorder or those youth whose early life 
symptoms are not easily obtained. Overall, 
the study�s superior specificity validated 
the DSM-5.21

However, different conclusions were 
found by McPartland et al4 in a sample of 
933 subjects from a previous DSM-IV field 
trial of which 657 were diagnosed with 
ASD. This group was evaluated with proposed 
DSM-5 criteria and sensitivity and 
specificity were measured. In this group, 
60.6% of ASD cases meet revised DSM-
5 criteria for ASD with a specificity of 
94.9%. Those with IQ > 70 and Asperger�s 
disorder were less likely to be diagnosed 
according to the DSM-5. For example, the 
sensitivity of the DSM-5 criteria to diagnose 
ASD in DSM-IV cases of Asperger�s 
disorder was only 25%. They concluded 
the new criteria could have detrimental 
clinical and research effect. Others have 
questioned the validity of their findings 
given the historical data and methods.1

Matson and colleagues22,23 have published 
several studies comparing the DSM-
IV with the DSM-5 and conclude the proposed 
changes will lead to 30% to 45% of 
children, adolescents, and adults classified 
with ASD per DSM-IV-TR to not meet criteria 
for ASD with DSM-5. 

The most recent study to date estimated 
how many children diagnosed with PDD 
or non-PDD using the DSM-IV-TR will 
no longer meet the necessary criteria for 
ASD under DSM-5. Using data from 4,453 
children with a clinical PDD diagnosis, 

TABLE 3. 

DSM-5 Proposed Criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorders

A. Persistent deficits in social communication and social interaction across contexts, not accounted 
for by general developmental delays, and manifest by all three of the following:

1. Deficits in social-emotional reciprocity; ranging from abnormal social approach and failure of 
normal back and forth conversation through reduced sharing of interests, emotions, and affect 
and response to total lack of initiation of social interaction.

2. Deficits in nonverbal communicative behaviors used for social interaction; ranging from 
poorly integrated verbal and nonverbal communication, through abnormalities in eye contact 
and body language, or deficits in understanding and use of nonverbal communication, to total 
lack of facial expression or gestures.

3. Deficits in developing and maintaining relationships, appropriate to developmental level (beyond 
those with caregivers); ranging from difficulties adjusting behavior to suit different social 
contexts through difficulties in sharing imaginative play and in making friends to an apparent 
absence of interest in people.

B. Restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities as manifested by at least two 
of the following:

1. Stereotyped or repetitive speech, motor movements, or use of objects; (such as simple motor 
stereotypies, echolalia, repetitive use of objects, or idiosyncratic phrases). 

2. Excessive adherence to routines, ritualized patterns of verbal or nonverbal behavior, or excessive 
resistance to change; (such as motoric rituals, insistence on same route or food, repetitive 
questioning or extreme distress at small changes).

3. Highly restricted, fixated interests that are abnormal in intensity or focus; (such as strong attachment 
to or preoccupation with unusual objects, excessively circumscribed or perseverative 
interests).

4. Hyper-or hypo-reactivity to sensory input or unusual interest in sensory aspects of environment; 
(such as apparent indifference to pain/heat/cold, adverse response to specific sounds or 
textures, excessive smelling or touching of objects, fascination with lights or spinning objects).

C. Symptoms must be present in early childhood (but may not become fully manifest until social 
demands exceed limited capacities)

D. Symptoms together limit and impair everyday functioning.

Source: American Psychiatric Association5



FEATURE

PEDIATRIC ANNALS 42:4 | APRIL 2013

and from 690 children with a non-PDD diagnosis, 
data from the Autism Diagnostic 
Interview, Revised (ADI-R) and Autism 
Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) 
was matched to the DSM-5 criteria. In this 
study, it was found that most children who 
received a diagnosis of one of the PDDs 
under the DSM-IV would receive the diagnosis 
of ASD under the DSM-5.23 

The overall sensitivity of the two were 
similar: the DSM-5 criteria identified 91% 
of children with a clinical DSM-IV diagnosis 
of PDD, with no change based on gender 
or IQ. The specificity was improved in 
the DSM-5, especially when impairment in 
social reciprocity and nonverbal behavior 
was required in both the parent report and 
the clinical observation. Children who did 
not meet the DSM-5 criteria did not demonstrate 
required impairments in social and 
communication functioning.24 To date, data 
from studies on the effectiveness of ASD 
diagnostic criteria have been retrospective. 
The most stringent have used diagnostic instruments 
based on the DSM-IV; however, 
the two studies with the largest sample size 
show the highest levels of sensitivity for 
DSM-5 (see Table 4). 

CALLS FOR MODIFICATION 
OF DSM-5

Modifications to the DSM-5 criteria 
already have been suggested to address 
these concerns of sensitivity. In addition 
to the proposals to reduce the criteria for 
social communication and interaction from 
three to two,4,19,21,23,24 there have also been 
proposals to change the number of RRB 
criteria from two to one.13,23 Another theory 
is that relaxing the onset criteria may 
improve the ability to detect early social 
interaction problems, thus improving sensitivity.
19,25 These changes would likely 
increase sensitivity while maintaining acceptable 
specificity.4,21 

Work on the DSM-5 continues, and as 
it nears publication more accurate estimates 
of sensitivity and specificity will be 
measured as criteria will be compared in 
vivo against DSM-IV criteria and the �gold 
standard� of expert diagnosis. Community 
and clinical populations will be assayed 
to provide current measures of sensitivity 
and specificity.1 

TABLE 4. 

Studies Comparing Diagnosis of ASD in DSM-IV and DSM-5 

Study / year

Number of Subjects

Type of Sample

Instruments Used

Results

Limitations

Mattila et al19 / 2011

82

Screened epidemiological 
sample 
diagnosed with DSM-
IV criteria

ADI-R, ADOS, early DSM-
5 criteria

DSM-5 was less sensitive 
than DSM-IV for ASD, 
(0.46)

Early DSM-5 criteria, 
prevalence for 
PDD-NOS was not 
examined

Mandy et al20 / 2012

708

Consecutive referrals 
to an autism specialty 
clinic

3Di

DSM-5 model was superior 
to DSM-IV

Higher functioning 
sample, 3Di is a DSM-
IV derived tool

Frazier et al21 / 2012

14,744 siblings 
(8,911 with autism)

Family-selected 
internet registry

Mapped caregiver rated 
SRS and SCQ to DSM-5 
criteria

DSM-5 had lower sensitivity, 
(0.81 vs. 0.95) but 
greater specificity, (0.97 
vs. 0.86) than DSM-IV

Early DSM-5 criteria, 
self-selected sample, 
reliance on caregiver 
reports only

McPartland et al4 / 
2012

933 (657 diagnosed 
with ASD)

Multicenter DSM-IV 
field trial database, 
with clear reliability 
data

Algorithm of items 
from DSM-IV mapped to 
match DSM-5 criteria

60.6% of cases with ASD 
met DSM-5 criteria with 
a specificity of 94.9%

Included only 48 
DSM-IV subjects with 
Asperger�s disorder, 
modified a historical 
data set to new criteria

Matson et al22,23 / 
2012

2,721 toddlers aged 
17-36 months

EarlySteps participants


Clinical judgment using 
diagnostic algorithms

52.2% of toddlers were 
diagnosed with ASD by 
DSM-5

Single author review 
of evaluations based 
on DSM-IV

Gibbs et al13 / 2012

132 youth

Referred to tertiary 
autism clinic for initial 
evaluation

ADOS, ADI-R

76.5% of participants 
were diagnosed with 
ASD by DSM-5

ADOS and ADI-R are 
DSM-IV based tools

Taheri and Perry33 
/ 2012

131 children aged 
2-12 years

Retrospective file 
review

CARS, DSM-IV checklist

62.6% of total sample 
met diagnosis of ASD by 
DSM-5

No Asperger�s disorder 
patients, DSM-5 
criteria were evaluated 
by checklist

Huerta et al24 / 2012

5,143 subjects, 
4453 had PDD

Data sets from family 
genetics study, 
university and autism 
center databases

ADI-R and ADOS 
matched to DSM-IV and 
DSM-5 criteria. Included 
parent report and/or 
direct observation

DSM-5 identified 91% 
of children with PDD 
diagnoses. Overall specificity 
was low, (0.53) but 
improved over DSM-IV

More severe clinical 
sample, retrospective 
data analysis





3Di = Developmental, Dimensional, and Diagnostic Interview; ADI-R = Autism Diagnostic Interview, ADOS = Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ASD = autism spectrum disorder; CARS = Childhood 
Autism Rating Scale; NOS = not otherwise specified; PDD = pervasive developmental disorder; Revised; SCQ = Social Communication Questionnaire; SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale. 



FEATURE

PEDIATRIC ANNALS 42:4 | APRIL 2013

Because it places primary focus on social 
communication problems, continued 
interest will explore the boundaries of normal 
and abnormal social communication. 
This is relevant because autistic traits appear 
to have some continuous dimension within 
a population.18 The field will need suitable 
instruments to measure social communication 
skills and determine distribution in the 
population, assign cutoff points, operationalize 
mild and moderate impairment, and 
measure adaptive function.6

CONCLUSION

Philosophical questions, such as whether 
autism is a single, continuous entity 
marked by impaired social interaction, will 
still exist after publication of the DSM-5. 
Some will point out the broad distribution 
of autistic symptoms in the population26 
and elevated rates in twins and siblings.
18,27 Conversely, there is the question 
of whether autism is a diverse collection 
of heterogeneous conditions with a shared 
group of symptoms. Then there are those 
who see autism as a collection of different 
syndromes and who point out that latent 
or emerging symptoms differentiate ASD 
from non-ASD children,28 a point of view 
supported by data on trajectory of brain 
development.29 The contrast between the 
two groups is impetus for improved understanding 
of autism. The DSM-5 fits squarely 
in the middle of these discussions.30 

The DSM-5 is the next evolutionary 
step for the diagnosis of autism based on 
the empirical input of the last 20 years of 
nosology and epidemiology.31,32 As with 
all diagnostic systems, it is a work in progress 
and is the best attempt so far to describe 
autism as we understand it today. 

REFERENCES

 1. Swedo SE, Baird G, Cook EH, Jr., et al. Commentary 
from the DSM-5 Workgroup on Neurodevelopmental 
Disorders. J Am Acad Child 
Adolesc Psychiatry. Apr 2012;51(4):347-349.

 2. Happe F. Criteria, categories, and continua: 
autism and related disorders in DSM-
5. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 
2011;50(6):540-542.

 3. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-5, 
Social Communication Disorder. 2012. Available 
at www.dsm5.org. Accessed March 6, 
2013.

 4. McPartland JC, Reichow B, Volkmar FR. 
Sensitivity and specificity of proposed DSM-
5 diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder. 
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 
2012;51(4):368-383. 

 5. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-5 
Development, Autism Spectrum Disorder. 
2012. Available at: www.dsm5.org. Accessed 
March 6, 2013.

 6. Tanguay PE. Autism in DSM-5. Am J Psychiatry. 
2011;168(11):1142-1144.

 7. Ritvo ER. Postponing the proposed changes 
in DSM-5 for autistic spectrum disorder until 
new scientific evidence adequately supports 
them. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(9):2021-
2022.

 8. Kanner L. Child Psychiatry. Springfield, IL: 
Charles C. Thomas; 1935.

 9. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic 
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 
3rd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric 
Association; 1980.

 10. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic 
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 
DSM-III-R. Washington, DC: American 
Psychiatric Association; 1987.

 11. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic 
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 
4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric 
Association; 1994.

 12. Lord C, Petkova E, Hus V, et alA multisite 
study of the clinical diagnosis of different autism 
spectrum disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 
2012;69(3):306-313.

 13. Gibbs V, Aldridge F, Chandler F, Witzlsperger 
E, Smith K. Brief Report: An Exploratory 
Study Comparing Diagnostic Outcomes for 
Autism Spectrum Disorders Under DSM-IV-
TR with the Proposed DSM-5 Revision. J Autism 
Dev Disord. 2012;42(8):1750-1756.

 14. Miller JN, Ozonoff S. The external validity of 
Asperger disorder: lack of evidence from the 
domain of neuropsychology. J Abnorm Psychol. 
2000;109(2):227-238.

 15. Gilchrist A, Green J, Cox A, Burton D, Rutter 
M, Le Couteur A. Development and current 
functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome: 
a comparative study. J Child Psychol 
Psychiatry. Feb 2001;42(2):227-240.

 16. Howlin P. Outcome in high-functioning adults 
with autism with and without early language 
delays: implications for the differentiation between 
autism and Asperger syndrome. J Autism 
Dev Disord. Feb 2003;33(1):3-13.

 17. Constantino JN, Hudziak JJ, Todd RD. Deficits 
in reciprocal social behavior in male 
twins: evidence for a genetically independent 
domain of psychopathology. J Am Acad Child 
Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42(4):458-467.

 18. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Autistic traits in 
the general population: a twin study. Arch 
Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(5):524-530.

 19. Mattila ML, Kielinen M, Linna SL, et al. Autism 
spectrum disorders according to DSM-
IV-TR and comparison with DSM-5 draft 
criteria: an epidemiological study. J Am Acad 
Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(6):583-
592.

 20. Mandy WP, Charman T, Skuse DH. Testing 
the construct validity of proposed criteria for 
DSM-5 autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad 
Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(1):41-50.

 21. Frazier TW, Youngstrom EA, Speer L, et al. 
Validation of proposed DSM-5 criteria for 
autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child 
Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(1):28-40.

 22. Matson JL, Kozlowski AM, Hattier MA, 
Horovitz M, Sipes M. DSM-IV vs DSM-5 diagnostic 
criteria for toddlers with autism. Dev 
Neurorehabil. 2012;15(3):185-190.

 23. Matson JL, Hattier MA, Williams LW. How 
Does Relaxing the Algorithm for Autism Affect 
DSM-V Prevalence Rates? J Autism Dev 
Disord. 2012;42(8):1549-1556.

 24. Huerta M, Bishop SL, Duncan A, Hus V, Lord 
C. Application of DSM-5 criteria for autism 
spectrum disorder to three samples of children 
with DSM-IV diagnoses of pervasive 
developmental disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 
2012;169(10):1056-1064.

 25. Wing L, Gould J, Gillberg C. Autism spectrum 
disorders in the DSM-V: better or 
worse than the DSM-IV? Res Dev Disabil. 
2011;32(2):768-773.

 26. Constantino JN. How continua converge in 
nature: cognition, social competence, and 
autistic syndromes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc 
Psychiatry. 2009;48(2):97-98.

 27. Constantino JN, Lajonchere C, Lutz M, et al. 
Autistic social impairment in the siblings of 
children with pervasive developmental disorders. 
Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(2):294-296.

 28. Frazier TW, Youngstrom EA, Sinclair L, et al. 
Autism spectrum disorders as a qualitatively 
distinct category from typical behavior in a 
large, clinically ascertained sample. Assessment. 
2010;17(3):308-320.

 29. Amaral DG, Schumann CM, Nordahl CW. 
Neuroanatomy of autism. Trend Neurosci. 
2008;31(3):137-145.

 30. Leventhal BL. Lumpers and splitters: who 
knows? Who cares? J Am Acad Child Adolesc 
Psychiatry. 2012;51(1):6-7.

 31. Ozonoff S. Editorial: DSM-5 and autism 
spectrum disorders - two decades of perspectives 
from the JCPP. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 
Jul 16 2012.

 32. Lord C, Jones RM. Annual research review: 
re-thinking the classification of autism spectrum 
disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 
2012;53(5):490-509.

 33. Taheri A, Perry A. Exploring the proposed 
DSM-5 criteria in a clinical sample. J Autism 
Dev Disord. 2012;42(9):1810-1817.